Can you be moral without religion




















The surprising truth about compassionate conservatism. Monkeys reject unequal pay. A cross-species perspective on the selfishness axiom. Behavioral and Brain Sciences , 28 , Grand Rapids, MI: Revell. Meme infection or religious niche construction? An adaptationist alternative to the cultural maladaptationist hypothesis.

Wilson D. The cultural evolution of religion In Richerson P. Engineering human cooperation: Does involuntary neural activation increase public goods contributions? Human Nature , 18 , 88— When god sanctions killing: Effect of scriptural violence on aggression. Psychological Science , 18 , — Copying results and copying actions in the process of social learning: Chimpanzees Pan troglodytes and human children Homo sapiens.

Animal Cognition , 8 , — An examination of religious priming and intrinsic religious motivation in the moral hypocrisy paradigm. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion , 48 , — Developmental continuity in teleo-functional explanation: Reasoning about nature among Romanian Romani adults.

Journal of Cognition and Development , 9 , — Pictures are worth a thousand words and a moral decision or two: Religious symbols prime moral judgments. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion.

The coevolution of parochial altruism and war. Religion, likelihood of action, and the morality of mentality. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion , 13 , — Religion and the morality of positive mentality.

Basic and Applied Social Psychology , 26 , 45—57 Religion and the morality of mentality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 81 , — Conceptualizing spirit possession: Ethnographic and experimental evidence. Ethos , 36 , — Cognitive Neuropsychology , 1 , 1—8 Delusional belief. Annual Review of Psychology , 62 , — Impulsive choice and altruistic punishment are correlated and increase in tandem with serotonin depletion. Emotion , 10 , — Europe: A history. London, UK: Pimlico.

The God delusion. London, UK: Transworld. Opposite-sex siblings decrease attraction, but not prosocial attributions, to self-resembling opposite-sex faces. Feith S. The neuropeptide oxytocin regulates parochial altruism in intergroup conflict among humans. London, UK: Penguin. The bright stuff. New York Times , p. Breaking the spell: Religion as a natural phenomenon. New York, NY: Viking. The brothers Karamazov Pevear R. The elementary forms of the religious life Swain J. Human cognitive neuropsychology.

Hove, UK: Erlbaum. Child Development , 85 , — Humble self-enhancement: Religiosity and the better-than-average effect. Social Psychological and Personality Science , 5 , 76—83 Effects of eye images on everyday cooperative behavior: A field experiment. Evolution and Human Behavior , 32 , — Patient and impatient punishers of free-riders.

Cognitive and contextual factors in the emergence of diverse belief systems: Creation versus evolution. Cognitive Psychology , 42 , — Altruistic punishment in humans. A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. The Quarterly Journal of Economics , , — Eyes are on us, but nobody cares: Are eye cues relevant for strong reciprocity? How do rituals affect cooperation? An experimental field study comparing nine ritual types. Human Nature , 24 , — Personal prayer buffers self-control depletion.

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology , 51 , 56—59 Collective action as a social exchange. Does religious belief promote prosociality? A critical examination. Psychological Bulletin , , — Rational imitation in preverbal infants.

Nature , , Finding the faithless: Perceived atheist prevalence reduces anti-atheist prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin , 37 , — In godlessness we distrust: Using social psychology to solve the puzzle of anti-atheist prejudice. Social and Personality Psychology Compass , 7 , — Perceiving minds and gods: How mind perception enables, constrains, and is triggered by belief in gods. Perspectives on Psychological Science , 8 , — Everything is permitted? People intuitively judge immorality as representative of atheists.

Good for God? Religious motivation reduces perceived responsibility for and morality of good deeds. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , , — Like a camera in the sky? Thinking about God increases public self-awareness and socially desirable responding. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology , 48 , — Psychological Science , 23 , — Religion and the origins of anti-atheist prejudice In Clarke S.

Do you believe in atheists? Distrust is central to anti-atheist prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , , — Religion and support for suicide attacks. Psychological Science , 20 , — The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: A critique of the adaptationist programme. Exaptation: A missing term in the science of form. Paleobiology , 8 , 4— Biological signals as handicaps. Journal of Theoretical Biology , , — Beyond beliefs: Religions bind individuals into moral communities.

Personality and Social Psychology Review , 14 , — Moral foundations theory: The pragmatic validity of moral pluralism. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology , 47 , 55— Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 96 , — The moral stereotypes of liberals and conservatives: Exaggeration of differences across the political spectrum. Mind perception is the essence of morality. Psychological Inquiry , 23 , — To give and to give not: The behavioral ecology of human food transfers.

Faces in the clouds: A new theory of religion. The Gods are watching: An experimental study of religion and traditional belief in Burkina Faso. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion , 51 , — The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion. New York, NY: Pantheon. When morality opposes justice: Conservatives have moral intuitions that liberals may not recognize.

Social Justice Research , 20 , 98— Planet of the Durkheimians, where community, authority, and sacredness are foundations of morality In Jost J. Intuitive ethics: How innately prepared intuitions generate culturally variable virtues. Daedalus , , 55—66 The moral mind: How 5 sets of innate moral intuitions guide the development of many culture-specific virtues, and perhaps even modules In Carruthers P. A mathematical theory of natural and artificial selection.

Mathematical Proceedings of the Cambridge Philosophical Society , 23 , — Subtle cues affect generosity in an anonymous economic game. Evolution and Human Behavior , 26 , — The genetical evolution of social behaviour. Journal of Theoretical Biology , 7 , 1—16 Give me strength or give me a reason: Self-control, religion, and the currency of reputation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences , 36 , — Love thy neighbor: The evolution of in-group morality.

Skeptic , 3 , 86— The sexual overperception bias: Evidence of a systematic bias in men from a survey of naturally occurring events. Journal of Research in Personality , 37 , 34—47 Error management theory: A new perspective on biases in cross-sex mind reading. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 78 , 81—91 The paranoid optimist: An integrative evolutionary model of cognitive biases. Personality and Social Psychology Review , 10 , 47—66 The evolution of costly displays, cooperation, and religion: Credibility enhancing displays and their implications for cultural evolution.

Evolution and Human Behavior , 30 , — Tracer D. Behavioral and Brain Sciences , 28 , — The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences , 33 , 61—83 Antisocial punishment across societies. Cognition , , — Why gods persist: A scientific approach to religion.

London, UK: Routledge. Modes theory: Some theoretical considerations In Whitehouse H. Was Tinbergen an Aristotelian? Human Ethology Bulletin , 28 , 3— Why immortality alone will not get me to the afterlife. Philosophical Psychology , 24 , — Social Cognition , 27 , — The archetypal actions of ritual: A theory of ritual illustrated by the Jain rite of worship.

Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press. Religion as a hard-to-fake sign of commitment In Nesse R. Understanding libertarian morality: The psychological dispositions of self-identified libertarians.

Berlin, Germany: Springer Physica-Verlag; Hand of God, mind of man: Punishment and cognition in the evolution of cooperation. Evolutionary Psychology , 4 , — The evolution of error: Error management, cognitive constraints, and adaptive decision-making biases.

The good of wrath: Supernatural punishment and the evolution of cooperation. Political Theology , 5 , — Priming Christian religious concepts increases racial prejudice. Social Psychological and Personality Science , 1 , — Experimental pretesting of hand-washing interventions in a natural setting.

American Journal of Public Health , 99 Suppl. Cognitive and neural foundations of religious belief. Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 3 , — The scope of teleological thinking in preschool children. Cognition , 70 , — Why are rocks pointy? Developmental Psychology , 35 , — Reasoning about purpose and design in nature. Psychological Science , 15 , — The human function compunction: Teleological explanation in adults.

Professional physical scientists display tenacious teleological tendencies: Purpose-based reasoning as a cognitive default. Over-imitation is better explained by norm learning than by distorted causal learning.

Proceedings of the Royal Society B , , — Diminishing reciprocal fairness by disrupting the right prefrontal cortex. Every step a lotus: Shoes for bound feet.

From is to ought: How to commit the naturalistic fallacy and get away with it in the study of moral development In Mischel T. The Nazi conscience. Cambridge, MA: Belknap. Dopamine, paranormal belief, and the detection of meaningful stimuli. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 22 , — A cue of kinship promotes cooperation for the public good.

Evolution and Human Behavior , 29 , 49—55 Differences in attitudes towards outgroups in religious and non-religious contexts in a multi-national sample: A situational context priming study. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion , 22 , 1—9 Cognitive preconditions for responses to fairness: An object retrieval test of inhibitory control in Capuchin Monkeys Cebus paella.

Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics , 2 , 12—20 Divergent effects of activating thoughts of God on self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , , 4—21 Religion and punishment: Opposing influences of orthopraxy and orthodoxy on reactions to unintentional acts.

Outsourcing punishment to God: Beliefs in divine control reduce earthly punishment. Young children are more generous when others are aware of their actions.

The architecture of human kin detection. When getting something good is bad: Even three-year-olds react to inequality. Social Development , 20 , — The hidden structure of overimitation. The effects of priming science on moral judgments and behavior. When Are religious people nicer? Judgment and Decision Making , 5 , — Hunter-gatherers and human evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology , 14 , 54—67 Cause and effect in biology: Kinds of causes, predictability, and teleology are viewed by a practicing biologist.

The dishonesty of honest people: A theory of self-concept maintenance. Journal of Marketing Research , 45 , — Why religion is natural and science is not. The subtleties of error management. Evolution and Human Behavior , 31 , — Wrath of God: Religious primes and punishment. The evolution of misbelief. Behavioral and Brain Sciences , 32 , — Language: A biological model. Inferences about mental states. A research note on religion and morality: Are religious people nice people?

Social Forces , 61 , — High heels as supernormal stimuli: How wearing high heels affects judgements of female attractiveness. The role of writing and recordkeeping in the cultural evolution of human cooperation. Ethnographic atlas. Standard cross-cultural sample. Ethnology , 8 , — Costly signaling and the origin of religion.

Journal of Cognition and Culture , 9 , — The young chimpanzee does not need an oracle to tell it to honor its mother and to refrain from killing its brothers and sisters. Of course, family squabbles and even murder have been observed in ape societies, but such behaviors are exceptions, not the norm. So too it is in human societies, everywhere and at all times. The African apes — whose genes are ninety-eight to ninety-nine percent identical to ours — go about their lives as social animals, cooperating in the living of life, entirely without the benefit of clergy and without the commandments of Exodus, Leviticus, or Deuteronomy.

It is further cheering to learn that sociobiologists have even observed altruistic behavior among troops of baboons. More than once, in troops attacked by leopards, aged, post reproduction-age males have been observed to linger at the rear of the escaping troop and to engage the leopard in what often amounts to a suicidal fight.

The heroism which we see acted out, from time to time, by our fellow men and women, is far older than their religions. Long before the gods were created by the fear-filled minds of our less courageous ancestors, heroism and acts of self-sacrificing love existed.

They did not require a supernatural excuse then, nor do they require one now. Given the general fact, then, that evolution has equipped us with nervous systems biased in favor of social, rather than antisocial, behaviors, is it not true, nevertheless, that antisocial behavior does exist, and it exists in amounts greater than a reasonable ethicist would find tolerable?

Alas, this is true. But it is true largely because we live in worlds far more complex than the Paleolithic world in which our nervous systems originated. To understand the ethical significance of this fact, we must digress a bit and review the evolutionary history of human behavior.

Today, heredity can control our behavior in only the most general of ways, it cannot dictate precise behaviors appropriate for infinitely varied circumstances. In our world, heredity needs help. In the world of a fruit fly, by contrast, the problems to be solved are few in number and highly predictable in nature. That is to say, most behaviors result from environmental activation of nerve circuits which are formed automatically by the time of emergence of the adult fly.

This is an extreme example of what is called instinctual behavior. Each behavior is coded for by a gene or genes which predispose the nervous system to develop certain types of circuits and not others, and where it is all but impossible to act contrary to the genetically predetermined script.

The world of a mammal — say a fox — is much more complex and unpredictable than that of the fruit fly. Consequently, the fox is born with only a portion of its neuronal circuitry hard-wired. That is, they may or may not hook up with each other in functional circuits, depending upon environmental circumstances.

Learned behavior is behavior which results from activation of these environmentally conditioned circuits. Learning allows the individual mammal to learn — by trial and error — greater numbers of adaptive behaviors than could be transmitted by heredity.

A fox would be wall-to-wall genes if all its behaviors were specified genetically. With the evolution of humans, however, environmental complexity increased out of all proportion to the genetic and neuronal changes distinguishing us from our simian ancestors. This partly was due to the fact that our species evolved in a geologic period of great climatic flux — the Ice Ages — and partly was due to the fact that our behaviors themselves began to change our environment.

The changed environment in turn created new problems to be solved. Their solutions further changed the environment, and so on. Thus, the discovery of fire led to the burning of trees and forests, which led to destruction of local water supplies and watersheds, which led to the development of architecture with which to build aqueducts, which led to laws concerning water-rights, which led to international strife, and on and on. Further, the lack of social consensus on many issues makes it impossible to equate ethics with whatever society accepts.

Some people accept abortion but many others do not. If being ethical were doing whatever society accepts, one would have to find an agreement on issues which does not, in fact, exist. What, then, is ethics? Ethics is two things. First, ethics refers to well-founded standards of right and wrong that prescribe what humans ought to do, usually in terms of rights, obligations, benefits to society, fairness, or specific virtues.

Ethics, for example, refers to those standards that impose the reasonable obligations to refrain from rape, stealing, murder, assault, slander, and fraud. Ethical standards also include those that enjoin virtues of honesty, compassion, and loyalty.

And, ethical standards include standards relating to rights, such as the right to life, the right to freedom from injury, and the right to privacy. Such standards are adequate standards of ethics because they are supported by consistent and well-founded reasons. Morals and religion these two words are often confused as interdependent entities which flow from one another, but the truth is quite different.

Let me start by stating the obvious first because though it is obviously still all the confusion stems out of the Understanding or May I say the misunderstanding of these two words. Religion as we know it today has evolved and has been codified over the centuries with changes in the social dynamics and social structure to suit the societal needs of contemporary times. A set of rules, norms, and conditions were spelled out to extract a favorable response from the practitioners of the religion towards the society.

These rules changed according to time, place, and more importantly convenience of the society as a whole. Though the religions of the world desire a favorable outcome, acceptable by the majority, the over-enthusiastic practitioners and implementers digressed from the novel origin to more dogmatic ways. Whenever a choice has been in front of the human the right and the wrong start weighing in his or her mind. Having said that, I feel that both the words seem to germinate out of each other.

It is actually the position or place from where the differentiator is looking at and on what ground the differentiator finds itself grounded. Now the question comes: Can morals exist without religion?



0コメント

  • 1000 / 1000